Case Report
Cognitive Stimulation of Autobiographic and Emotional Memory in a Patient with Alzheimer’s Disease
Bruno D*, Suppicich F, Schurmann Vignaga S, Klein A, Aiskovich M, Torralva T, Manes F, Pontello N and Roca M
Corresponding Author: Bruno Diana, Av. José Ignacio de la Roza 1516, J5400 San Juan, Rivadavia, Argentina
Received: February 04, 2020; Accepted: February 17, 2020 Available Online: July 07, 2020
Citation: Bruno D, Suppicich F, Vignaga SS, Klein A, Aiskovich M, et al. (2021) Cognitive Stimulation of Autobiographic and Emotional Memory in a Patient with Alzheimer’s Disease. J Psychiatry Psychol Res, 4(1): 210-218.
Copyrights: ©2021 Bruno D, Suppicich F, Vignaga SS, Klein A, Aiskovich M, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Share :
  • 1033

    Views & Citations
  • 33

    Likes & Shares

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the most frequent cause of dementia for people over 65 years of age. AD is characterized by a progressive cognitive decline that generally begins with deficits in the anterograde memory and then evolves to a general cognitive deterioration. The decline of autobiographical memory in AD leads to a loss of knowledge about events that define patients’ lives and consequently, causes a degradation of self-knowledge and sense of identity. These difficulties compromise the patient’s autonomy, causing a decrease in the quality of life of patients and their caregivers. To promote cognition, independence, and wellbeing in AD’s patients medical and pharmacological treatments should be complemented by non-pharmacological interventions. The use of new technologies in non-pharmacological treatments is gaining importance. However; there are few reports on the effectiveness of these strategies. The aim of this study is to evaluate the impact of the use of autobiographical training software (ATS) on autobiographical and emotional memory in an AD’s patient. In the case reported, the use of the application stimulated autobiographical remembering, thus serving as a scaffolding toll for the reconstruction of semantic autobiographical memory.

 

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, Cognitive stimulation, Autobiographic memory, Emotional memory, Training software

INTRODUCTION

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most frequent cause of dementia in people over 65 years of age. [1-3] Currently, there are over 36.5 million people in the world who are affected by dementia and the majority of these cases are AD-related [2,4-8]. It is estimated that the number of cases will double in the next 20 years [9].

AD is a progressive neurodegenerative disease that usually begins after age 65. AD is characterized by increasing decrements in recent memory, language, visuospatial and executive functions [10,3]. The progressive cognitive decline in AD usually begins with deficits in the anterograde memory and culminates with global cognitive deterioration [11]. These difficulties compromise the patient’s autonomy, causing a decrease in their quality of life and that of their caregivers [12]. The etiology of AD remains unknown, although it is considered a disease of multifactorial causes, where the main non-modifiable risk factor is age [13,14].

While the hallmark symptom of AD is the decline in episodic memory, as the disease progresses, other memory systems - including autobiographical memory - are also typically affected. Autobiographical memory is defined as the memory of personal experiences and the facts which allow individuals to define themselves, build their life stories [15] and attribute meaning to their lives. It has been proposed that autobiographical memory has a semantic and an episodic component [16]. While the semantic component is made up of the general representations of life (cover long lifetime periods and general events referring to thematic events that occur repeatedly) [17], the episodic component is involved specific experiences that are identifiable in time and in normal aging, autobiographical episodic memory is negatively affected, while autobiographical semantic memory is usually preserved [18].

The decline of autobiographical memory in AD leads to a loss of knowledge about events that define patient’s lives and consequently, causes a degradation of self-knowledge and sense of identity. As autobiographical memory declines, it affects the activities of daily life, causing a negative impact on the patient’s quality of life [19]. Consistent with what is observed in normal ageing and with the decline of episodic memory; patients with AD reconstruct less autobiographical episodic memories and have over-dependence on autobiographical semantic memory [15]. It has been proposed that since autobiographical semantic memory is the features of memory that is best preserved over time, it can be used as a scaffolding to support the person sense of identity, accompanied by other cognitive strategies [20]. It is important to mention that emotions trigger anterograde memory [16]. As mentioned above, while the decline of episodic memory is the hallmark of AD disease, emotional processing and emotional memory are relatively preserved in the early stages of the disease [21]. Previous studies demonstrated in patients with amnesia that emotions can persist regardless of the episodic memory that caused the emotion itself [22]. Thus, AD patients can report emotional states while experiencing difficulties in the evocation of the episodic memory of initially triggered such emotion [23]. In other words, emotions persist even when the memory linked to that emotion has disappeared, a phenomenon that has been labeled by some authors as “emotions without memory” [24]. International guidelines suggest that in patients with AD, medical and pharmacological treatments should be complemented by non-pharmacological interventions [25]. Among such intervention multidisciplinary treatments that include caregivers have been reported to have a good level of effectiveness in patients with AD [25]. Approaches aimed at improving cognition such as cognitive training and stimulation activities of daily living (ADL) have reported high levels of effectiveness in mood and quality of life [26]. Along these lines, controlled and randomized research shows that training is not only effective when it is carried out individually, but also when the family is part of that process [27,28]. To endorse cognition, independence and wellbeing in AD’ patient the recommendations of the UK’s National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) offer a range of activities tailored to the individual preferences of those living with dementia and offer group cognitive stimulation therapy [29].

Other non- pharmacological treatments include the use of modern technology. Mobile phones, smartphones and tablets provide exceptional means for the application of mobile health to AD patients. In fact, most of the applications available -both in the literature and commercially- have been developed for the treatment of AD and other type dementia. [30] These instruments include stimulation games used in a preventive manner, applications to emotionally support patients and their caregivers, as well as applications to check the progress of dementia. Other applications fall under the category of serious games, that is specialized digital applications that aim at education, information, communication and stimulation of different cognitive domains [31]. To our knowledge, serious games that have as an objective the stimulation of autobiographical memory or the use of the emotions to trigger anterograde memory, specifically in patients with AD have not been previously studied. In this study we designed and implement Autobiographic Training Software (ATS) on a patient with AD. The purpose of this software is to stimulate autobiographical memory through the patient’s own life material (photos, videos and music) and is specifically designed to be used by patient with cognitive impairment. One of the advantages of this software is that can be adapted to the patient need and performance.

MATERIAL & METHODS

Case study

Our patient, RT, is a 72-year-old male with a presumptive diagnosis of AD, documented both in neuropsychological evaluations and imaging studies (hippocampal atrophy-Figure 1). RT completed his graduate studies as a public accountant and worked for more than 20 years at an international pharmaceutical company. His job required him to travel worldwide and learn different languages. Important aspects in his life are his love for his grandchildren, traveling, photography, listening to music and playing billiards. The first symptoms of dementia, according to both RT and his family, appeared at 66 years old and included episodes of confusion, temporary disorientation, attentional failures and a tendency to repeat the same comments and questions. Although at the beginning the clinical picture was consistent with a mood disorder, after an abrupt and unexpected retirement, it evolved into one more consistent with dementia, AD was diagnosed 5 years later. Nowadays, RT’s wife report to lack of initiative and frequent episodes of spatial disorientation, together with important alterations in episodic and autobiographical memory (particularly related to the most recent decades of his life). RT’s wife gave her signed informed consent for her husband’s participation in the study. The consent was obtained according to the terms of the Declaration of Helsinki.

At the beginning of this research study, RT was evaluated at a general cognitive level with the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III, [32,33] which showed a general cognitive decline, particularly in episodic memory and verbal fluency tasks. RT obtains a score of 60, which is 26 points below the suggested cut-off point, denoting an important cognitive compromise. He was also evaluated with the Ineco Frontal Screening [34] also showing executive dysfunction. RT obtains a score of 16, which is 9 points below the suggested cut-off point, denoting an important compromise in executive functions (Table 1).

Neuropsychological assessment

In order to evaluate the effect of the ATS on autobiographical memory, RT was assessed before and after the intervention with two different scales: The Modified Screening of Autobiographical Memory and the Autobiographical Memory Test.

 

The Modified Screening of Autobiographical Memory [18] consists of the recording of autobiographical memories segmented in periods of the patient’s life. The memories are categorized according to their episodic content, taking into account the time, space, thoughts and emotions connected of each memory. The results obtained by RT in this test can be seen in Table 2.

 

The Autobiographical Memory Test [35] is made up of 15 words that present positive, negative or neutral valence. The test requires the patient to evoke memories connected to words previously read. This memories are divided into 5 categories a) specific memories (events occurred at a specific time and place), b) extended memories: covering relatively periods of time (weeks, moth), c) categorical memory: repetitive events, d) memory of semantic association: when a specific event is not mentioned, but there is an association with a target word and e) omissions: when the patient that’s not react to the stimuli or repeats an answer. The results obtained by RT in this test can be seen in Table 3.

 

In the following stage of this study, the patient was provided with a tablet containing the ATS developed by the research team. Two additional visual analogue scales were applied in order to in order to assess the ease of use of the instrument and overall user’s satisfaction, respectively. The first scale measures eleven aspects with a liker response scale that ranges from 0 (bad) to 10 (excellent). The second scale explores measures nine elements in a similar response format, from 0 (nothing) to 10 (a lot). Both, the patient and his wife, were asked to report both scales. The results can be seen in Tables 4 and 5.

Intervention: The autobiographic training software (ATS)

Description of the software

The Autobiographical Training Software (ATS) was designed to stimulate autobiographical memory from personalized visual and musical stimuli in patients with AD. The software designed team included an information technology (IT) specialist, a graphic designer, a neuropsychologist and an occupational therapist. The IT specialist wrote the software program for the ATS on PHP and JavaScript platforms. The graphic designer set up a simple, easy-to-use interface, appropriate to the patient's limitations. The neuropsychologist and the occupational therapist provided information regarding AD and RT's cognitive profile. As a result and considering the patient's cognitive difficulties, the software interface included large fonts (for readability) and few menu options (to reduce the potential for confusion). All this aimed at increasing patient's autonomy regarding the use of the tablet at home. Moreover, the application can automatically change the difficulty level of the exercises according to the patient’s performance.

The application has two sections. The first one is observational, in which there is a timeline with photographs of the patient, his family and relevant events of his life. Photos may be organized and presented by decades or by life stages: childhood, youth, adulthood and present time. The patient looks at the photographs and has the option to either generate a memory or write a comment about them. The written comment is available for reading aloud the next time the same photo is viewed. The second section of the application allows for a more active role from the patient, as it includes games based on the patient’s life (front page ATS -Figure 2).

The games selected are the following:

·       Puzzle: The patient must re-assemble a photo of himself/herself.

·       Quiz: A set of questions about details of the patient’s personal life, as well as general information.

·       Hangman: A guessing game in which the patient must guess a word based on a photograph.

 

·       Word soup: The patient must search for familiar words between letters.

The games selected are the following:

·       Puzzle: The patient must re-assemble a photo of himself/herself.

·       Quiz: A set of questions about details of the patient’s personal life, as well as general information.

·       Hangman: A guessing game in which the patient must guess a word based on a photograph.

·       Word soup: The patient must search for familiar words between letters.

Training

The training for ATS lasted a total of 6 months. It consisted of weekly individual sessions of Occupational Therapy focused on use of the tablet in the context of the ATS Application. The first month of intervention was devoted to identifying, necessary adaptations and modifications to the requirements of the application to the patient’s level of general functioning. After that, the development team worked on patient’s access to the technological device, as well as the use of the different activities and tasks offered by the application. RT had to use the application at least five times a week, for a period of at least 15 min at a time.

RESULTS

After 6 months of training, improvements were observed both in the autobiographical memory tests and in the visual analogue scale. In the Modified Screening Test of Autobiographic Memory, no significant differences were observed between the scores obtained before and after stimulation with the ATS application, respectively (Table 2). However, the patient recalled more memories for the age range 20-29.

In the Autobiographic Memory Test (Table 3) we observed similar results between pre- and post-training performances. However, while specific and extended memory worsened, categorical memory improved after the 6 months of training. The latter was related to the semantic component of autobiographical memory. Qualitatively, it was observed that after the training both words of negative (eg. illness) and positive valence (eg. friendship) generated emotional reactions which accompanied the recall.

In the visual analogue scale of Tablet Management (Table 4) RT showed some improvements. The patient also reported increasingly extended period of time using the tablet and higher frequency of use of the device.

In the visual analogue scale of satisfaction level (Table 5), RT reported greater satisfaction in relation to music, more interest in the use of the tablet and greater motivation to look at photos of his family and to perform recreational activities.

DISCUSSION

The deterioration of autobiographical memory has a very important impact on the daily life of patients and their families. Previous studies have shown that episodic autobiographical memories begin to deteriorate in normal aging, leading to a dependence on semantic autobiographical memories. Therefore, it has been proposed that semantic autobiographical memory can be used as scaffolding to improve the patient’s identity, accompanied with other cognitive strategies [16]. In AD, this phenomenon is observed to a greater extent than in normal aging. This makes it challenging for professionals to stimulate patient’s memory without the patient’s personal material. There is nowadays a variety of serious games whose objective is cognitive stimulation, fundamentally in the domain of memory and presenting general material for all types of patients. Many of these games are too complex for people with AD to easily use. Until now, no serious games have been proposed for the stimulation of autobiographical memory allowing the patient to use their own material to enhance recall.

In the present study, we investigated the effect of ATS as a serious game to stimulate autobiographical and emotional memory for a period of 6 months in a patient of 72 years with a presumptive diagnosis of AD. From the results described, the application seemed to stimulate the autobiographical memory, serving as scaffolding for the semantic autobiographical memory and improving the recall of emotional contents. Although there was no greater recall of memories, more semantic-type memories were observed, accompanied by a higher emotional content in the memories recalled (memories accompanied by laughter and crying, unlike the evaluations made prior to training). It is inferred from this, that the training in the use of ATS and the constant contact of the patient with significant information from the present and past may have had an impact on the subject’s emotional memory. In the case of RT, the use of the application allowed to favor its autobiographic memory based on semantic aspects, even if does not maintain episodic memories. This was made possible by the use of own audiovisual material during the exercise, that produce a high impact at the level of personal and emotional identity. Additionally, the quality of life of RT and his family was favored, an important aspect to consider when working with patients with Alzheimer's dementia.

CONCLUSION

This study allowed us to present ATS as a useful tool for stimulating autobiographical and emotional memory in patients with AD and strengthening semantic autobiographical memories.

It is important to mention some limitations inherent to the study; the first is that it is a unique case, so the results cannot be generalized. The second one that was not applied a pre-post-classic classic neuropsychological battery, only screening tests and specific autobiographical memory tests were administered, this limited the possible analyzes. Further research is necessary to evaluate the impact of ATS in other patients with AD and other pathologies that involve autobiographical episodic memory, as well as to assess the effects of ATS over longer periods of time.

DISCLOSURE

 

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.

1.     Jack Jr CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, Carrillo MC, Dunn     B, et al. (2018) NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement 14: 535-562.

2.     Sosa-Ortiz AL, Acosta-Castillo I, Prince MJ (2012) Epidemiology of dementias and Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Med Res 43: 600-608.

3.     Salachevsky A, Parrao T, Oyarzo F, Lillo P (2019) Las Demencias: Historia, concepto, clasificación y aproximación clínica. En E. Labos, A, Slachevsky., T, Torralva., P. Fuentes, & F. Manes, Tratado de Neuropsicología Clínica 2019, pp: 652-679. Buenos Aires: Akadia.

4.     Qiu C, Kivipelto M, von Strauss E (2009) Epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: Occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 11: 111-128.

5.     Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, et al. (2005) Global prevalence of dementia: A Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366: 2112-2117.

6.     Wimo A, Winblad B, Aguero-Torres H, von Strauss E (2003) The magnitude of dementia occurrence in the world. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 17: 63-67.

7.     Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM (2007) Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer's Dement 3: 186-191.

8.     Robinson M, Lee BY, Hane FT (2017) Recent progress in Alzheimer’s disease research, Part 2: Genetics and epidemiology. J Alzheimer’s Dis 57: 317-330.

9.     Abbott A (2011) Dementia: A problem for our age. Nature 475: S2-S4.

10. Bondi MW, Jak AJ, Delano-Wood L, Jacobson MW, Delis DC, et al. (2008) Neuropsychological contributions to the early identification of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychol Rev 18: 73-90.

11. Howieson DB, Dame AD, Comicioli R, Sexton G, Payami H, et al. (1997) Cognitive markers preceding Alzheimer ́s disease in the healthy oldest old. J Am Geriatr Soc 45: 584-589.

12. Tromp D, Despres O, Dufour A, Lithfous S, Pebayle T, et al. (2015) Episodic memory in normal aging and Alzheimer Disease: Insights from imaging and behavioral studies. Ageing Res Rev 24: 232-262.

13. Sindi S, Mangialasche F, Kivipelto M (2015) Advances in the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease. Prime Reports 7: 50-62.

14. Valls-Pedret C, Molinuevo JL, Rami L (2010) Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Prodromal and preclinical phase. Revista de Neurología 51: 471-480.

15. Jetten J, Haslam C, Pugliese C, Tonks J, Haslam SA (2010) Declining autobiographical memory and the loss of identity: Effects on well-being. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32: 408-416.

16. El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Kapogiannis D (2015) Autobiographical memory decline in Alzheimer's disease, a theoretical and clinical overview. Ageing Res Rev 23: 183-192.

17. Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. Organization of memory. New York: Academic Pres, pp: 381-403.

18. Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M (2002) Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging 17: 677-689.

19. Surr CA (2006) Preservation of self in people with dementia living in residential care: A socio-biographical approach. Soc Sci Med 62: 1720-1730.

20. Barnabe A, Whitehead V, Pilon R, Arsenault-Lapierre G, Chertkow H (2012) Autobiographical memory in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: A comparison between the Levine and Kopelman interview methodologies. Hippocampus 22: 1809-1825.

21. Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW (2000) The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev 107: 261-288.

22. Broster LS, Blonder LX, Jiang Y (2012) Does emotional memory enhancement assist the memory-impaired? Front Aging Neurosci 4: 2.

23. Feinstein JS, Duff MC, Tranel D (2010) Sustained experience of emotion after loss of memory in patients with amnesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 7674-7679.

24. Guzmán-Vélez E, Feinstein JS, Tranel D (2010) Feelings without memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Cogn Behav Neurol 27: 117-129.

25. Woods B, Aguirre E, Spector AE, Orrell M (2012) Cognitive stimulation to improve cognitive functioning in people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 15: CD005562.

26. Olazarán J, Reisberg B, Clare L, Cruz I, Peña-Casanova J, et al. (2010) Nonpharmacological therapies in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review of efficacy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 30:161-178.

27. Quayhagen MP, Quayhagen M, Corbeil RR, Roth PA, Rodgers JA (1995) A dyadic remediation program for care recipients with dementia. Nurs Res 44: 153-159.

28. Quayhagen M, Quayhagen M, Corbeil RR, Hendrix RC, Jackson JE, et al. (2000) Coping with dementia: Evaluation of four nonpharmacologic interventions. Int Psychogeriatr 12: 249-265.

29. Pink J, O’Brien J, Robinson L, Longson D (2018) Dementia: Assessment, management and support: Summary of updated NICE guidance. BMJ 361: k2438.

30. Martínez-Pérez B, de la Torre-Díez I, López-Coronado M, sainz-de-Abajo B, Robles M, et al. (2014) Mobile clinical decision support systems and applications: A literature and commercial review. J Med Syst 38: 4.

31. Robert PH, König A, Amieva H, Andrieu S, Bremond F, et al. (2014) Recommendations for the use of serious games in people with Alzheimer’s disease, related disorders and frailty. Front Aging Neurosci 54: 1-13.

32. Hsieh S, Schubert S, Hoon C, Mioshi E, Hodges JR (2013) Validation of the Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 36: 242-250.

33. Bruno D, Slachevsky A, Fiorentino N, Rueda DS, Bruno G, et al. (2017) Argentinian/Chilean validation of the Spanish-language version of Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III for diagnosing dementia. Neurologia.

34. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, López P, Manes F (2009) INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): A brief, sensitive and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 15: 777-786.

 

35. Trives JJR, Postigo JML, Segura LR (2013) Design and analysis of the functioning of the Autobiographical Memory Test in the Spanish population. Apuntes de Psicología 31: 3-10.